Where u at? Teenagers, Texting and the Road: A Critique of the Public Health Response to Distracted Driving – Shannon McAuliffe
I have so many things to do today! I need
to go pick up my clothes from the cleaner, I have to get a birthday card for my
sister, I have to drop off…oh wait, is that my phone ringing? It must be in my
purse somewhere...
We all know how easy it is to become
distracted while driving. Our mind shuffles through all the things we need to
get accomplished in the next couple of hours while we struggle to find our
ringing phone at the bottom of a bag without missing the green light. It is
estimated that distracted driving played a role in 1,090,748 motor vehicle
crashes in 2009 in the USA; 385,910 of these accidents were estimated to
involve teenagers (1). Distracted driving can occur in three forms: visual,
manual and cognitive (1). Visual distraction involves taking one’s eyes off the
road, manual distraction involves taking one’s hands off the wheel and
cognitive distraction involves taking one’s mind off the task of driving (1). While
distracted drivers are 23 times more likely to get in to a car accident that
those who are focused on driving, teen drivers are already 4 times more likely
to get in to an accident than more experienced drivers (2). A study done at the
University of Utah found that using a cell phone while driving, regardless of
whether it is hands-held or hands-free, has the same effect on a driver’s
reaction time as a blood alcohol concentration of .08%, the legal limit (3). By
combining the inexperience of young drivers with the distractions provided by a
cellular phone, tragedy can ensue.
Text messaging has become the primary
means of communication for young adults, as a recent study of 13-19 year olds
found that over 78% reported using their phone more for texting than for
placing calls (4). In fact, during the month of June 2011, more than 196
billion text messages were sent or received in the United States; this was up
nearly 50% from June 2009 (5). Sending or receiving a text message strengthens
a teen’s social network, assuring them that they are still connected to their
peers even if they may not be in the same place at the same time. Parents can
also feel comforted by the fact that their child is only a phone call away if
they run into any problems, on the road or elsewhere. However, with the
presence of a cell phone in the car, it can be difficult for a teen to ignore
an incoming call or text, regardless of whether they are stopped at a red light
or cruising down the highway. Texting while driving combines all three types of
distraction described previously (1). Not only is the driver manually distracted
when they take their hand off the wheel to pick up their phone, they are also cognitively
distracted when they think about what the incoming call or text could say and
visually distracted when they look at the screen instead of the road. When
sending or receiving a text and traveling at 55 miles per hour, the driver’s
eyes are pulled from the road for an average of 4.6 seconds; the distance traveled
at this speed in this amount of time equals the length of a football field (5).
Essentially, that driver is careening down the road, blinded to what lies
before, behind or to the side of them, for almost 5 seconds.
Currently, the use of all cell phone
functions by new drivers is restricted in 31 states and the District of
Columbia while text messaging is banned for all drivers in 37 states and the
District of Columbia (6). The U.S. Department of Transportation has also joined
the fight to eliminate distracted driving. Led by Secretary of Transportation
Ray LaHood, a website has been created to address the frightening facts about
distracted driving and what can be done to prevent its dangers. Distraction.gov
provides the explicit statistics about the dangers of cell phone use while
driving, as well as tools for employers, educators and parents to use to talk
to the teens in their care about the hazards of driving while distracted.
Distraction.gov struggles to create an
effective public health campaign addressing the dangers of texting while
driving for a number of reasons. While motor vehicle crashes are the leading
cause of death for teenagers in the US (7), Distraction.gov strongly implies
that teens are the primary perpetrators of distracted driving. In a message
written by Senator LaHood in a recent brochure, he states that “…teens are
especially vulnerable because of their inexperience behind the wheel and,
often, peer pressure,” (7). While this may be accurate, is this the most
appropriate way to begin a discussion with teens about the dangers of driving
while distracted? By instructing parents and educators to talk to their kids
about the dangers of texting or calling while driving, this campaign paints teens
as the only guilty party who must be instructed by their elders in order to
understand the complexities of such an issue. Teens may feel targeted, treated
as the only suspects with the rest of their communities labeled as the
potential victims. Through its lack of appreciation for the Theory of
Psychological Reactance and the Theory of Optimistic Bias, as well as its
misuse of the Theory of Planned Behavior, Distraction.gov cannot serve as a
source respected by teenagers and parents alike in the fight against distracted
driving.
Criticism 1: “I’ll do what
I want to do!”
The teenage years are already known to
create tension between adolescents and their parents. As teenagers learn to
develop their own opinions and understand what they like and do not like, they
strive to assert their independence and be in control of their own lives. Teens
want to stay out later at night, go to parties where adults will not be present
and wear the latest questionable fashions, regardless of what their parents say
or feel. With a brand-new license in hand and many roads to explore,
discussions between parents and their new drivers may become even more
combative. A parent is concerned about the safety of their child and wants to
make sure that they will take all of the right precautions when they are on the
road by themselves for the first time, whereas their teen just cannot wait to
get in to the driver’s seat. When parents tell their son or daughter to keep
the phone out of their hands while driving, it can be the last thing a young
driver remembers when they get behind the wheel.
Distraction.gov plays a similar role in the
life of a teen driver as do the teen’s own parents. By using slogans such as
“Stop Texts, Stop Wrecks” and “No one is jk or LOL now,” the intervention
functions by telling teens what they should not do and by playing a patronizing
role (2). Telling teens that if they stop texting car accidents will not occur
can be seen as an omnipresent superior telling the young driver what to do.
When the intervention uses the slang terms “jk” and “LOL,” it can be seen as an
adult speaking down to an adolescent by mimicking social jargon. Both of these examples can serve to incite
psychological reactance in teen viewers. The theory of psychological reactance
is based on the concept that when someone is told what to do and they find
their freedom restricted, they will respond negatively and do whatever they can
to reestablish their control (8). Described as the “boomerang effect,” when a
freedom is limited, people can respond by choosing to exercise that freedom (9).
As teenagers are known to do whatever
their parents tell them not to do, campaigns that incite similar reactions in
young drivers may not be successful as they infringe upon the apparent freedom
of teens. When the messenger is the government, moreover, the message may not
be well perceived by teenagers; they would be more likely to respond to a
source that they felt they could relate to, such as one of their peers (10).
Criticism 2: “But it would
never happen to ME…”
Teenagers can be bombarded daily with
warnings about what dangers await them around every corner. If you use a tanning bed, you have an
increased risk of getting melanoma. If you drink and drive, you can kill
yourself and others. If you smoke, you
can develop lung cancer or emphysema. Even though they know the statistics
and facts, how many teens take these precautions to heart? While they may
understand the risks associated with getting in a car with a driver who has
been drinking or not practicing abstinence or using birth control, adolescents
tend to believe they are invincible and that only good things will happen to
them.
The Theory of Optimistic Bias explains
the idea that we tend to underestimate the chance that bad things are going to
happen to us personally while overestimating the chance that good things will
happen to us (11). This is not only true regarding teenagers; adults can also
be unrealistically optimistic about the future while overlooking the
possibility that a negative event will affect them.
The current government intervention shared
through Distraction.gov provides statistics about the dangers of texting while
driving. The site recommends that parents and educators talk with the teens in
their care about these alarming statistics, insinuating that the problem lies
in adolescents not understand the dangers of using a cell phone while driving.
However, teenagers know how dangerous texting while driving can be; in a recent
study, 84% of teen drivers said they were aware that distracted driving could
increase their risk of being involved in a car accident (2). Through the
optimistic bias, they understand the risk of their actions but do not think it
will apply to them. One study showed that 4 in 5 young drivers admit they have
texted while driving (2), while other studies reveal that up to 91.2% of
college drivers admitted to texting while driving (12). It is evident that
frightening facts and statistics about the perils of distracted driving have
done little to curb such behaviors in young drivers who believe that they have
their whole lives ahead of them.
Criticism 3: The Theory of
Planned Behavior…Gone Wrong
A number of the suggestions posited by Distraction.gov
revolve around the Theory of Planned Behavior, which focuses on the rational
decision-making processes involved in human risk assessment (13). This theory
explains how, as rational human beings, we think about our actions before we
perform them, though we may not always act based on our personal attitudes (13).
The intention to act is determined by a number of factors, such as the person’s
perceived behavioral control (whether they believe they will be able to perform
that action), perceived power (the amount of power that the person believes
they have over performing that action), as well as subjective norms (what the
person thinks their peers and social groups will think of the action) (13).
As described previously, the behavior of
texting while driving in the teen population is not reflective of their lack of
knowledge regarding this behavior’s dangers. Adolescents understand that
texting while driving is not a safe activity to engage in, although this
knowledge does not prevent them from performing that behavior. With the Theory
of Planned Behavior, it is believed that teens will make the rational decision
not to text while driving simply because they understand its hazards and
because they have the ability and power not to text while driving. This is
clearly an incorrect assumption that can have deadly consequences. In fact, it
has been shown that past behaviors are one of the strongest predictors of the
future intention and behavior of a person (14); this implies that over 90% of
teens will text while driving in the future (12).
Distraction.gov strongly emphasizes that
the power to stop texting and calling while driving is available to all of us.
Any teen can make the decision that they will not use a cell phone while behind
the wheel; the website even provides a pledge teenagers can sign, guaranteeing
that they will
“Protect lives by never texting or talking on the phone while driving; Be a
good passenger and speak out if the driver in my car is distracted; Encourage
my friends and family to drive phone-free,” (15). However, it is clear that
having this power and knowing the statistics does not ensure that a teen will
not pick up the phone while they are driving.
Encouraging the use of social media sites
such as Facebook and Twitter to spread the word to friends about the risks of
using a cell phone while driving serves to address the social norms component
of the Theory of Planned Behavior. By designing competitions such as the
Distracted Driving Design Challenge, which asks high school students to design
an icon representing distracted driving that can be shared on social media
sites, Distraction.gov addresses the importance of social networking and how
critical it is to the life of a teen (16). In a “Toolkit” segment of the
website, teens can even print up posters that say “iSmash my car because iUsed
my phone while driving” or “iCrash because iCalled my boyfriend while driving”,
playing off the visible marketing of Apple products that so many teens own and
use on a daily basis (17).
The website also provides startling
statistics about the prevalence of texting while driving in the teen community.
While this may seem like an excellent way to get adolescents to understand the
gravity of the issue, by illustrating the ubiquitous nature of the problem,
texting while driving is seen as the social norm and, therefore, can actually
encourage more teens to text while driving. As discussed in a study out of
Australia, public health interventions should “…[minimize] the perceived
texting frequency of other young people, [endeavoring] to portray texting while
driving as a [behavior] which few young adults engage in or approve of,” (14).
Overall, Distraction.gov creates a truly
well-planned campaign based off of the Theory of Planned Behavior by addressing
each of its essential components and providing plenty of documentation to
support the development of healthy behavioral intentions. Although the
intervention may be well-designed, it is still not effective. In fact, a recent
study found that the Theory of Planned Behavior only accounted for a small
percentage (11-14%) of the variance in people’s intention to text while driving (14). There must be other factors that are influencing
to teens to engage in such risky behaviors that cannot be explained by this
theory or quelled by the intervention designed by Distraction.gov.
So Where Do We Go From
Here? Employing New Strategies
How
do we design a public health campaign that adequately addresses the concerns of
parents and lawmakers while appealing to the adolescent population? Designing a
campaign for teenagers is not like designing a campaign that is meant to target
adults. As previously discussed, adolescents are extremely social yet fragile
human beings who react differently to public health tactics than their parents
and older role models. By using the previous criticisms to mold an intervention
that minimizes psychological reactance and combats the optimistic bias, it is feasible
to create a campaign that truly challenges the social norm and incites change
in the teenage population.
To avoid the psychological reactance that is
so present in current public health campaigns, an intervention must strive for
explicitness and reason while avoiding dominance and putting the control back
in the hands of the audience (10). While
Distraction.gov is certainly explicit in its presentation of the issue, there
is a high level of dominance that can overtake the meaning behind the message.
A website created by a government entity is hardly a source that a teen would
pay attention to when there are so many other sites they would rather be
visiting. It would be more effective to use a source that is similar to the
target audience and in a forum that the target audience would commonly visit.
Optimistic bias is a difficult obstacle
to overcome in the carrying out of public health campaigns. Regardless of the
very real and frightening reports of motor vehicle crashes caused by distracted
driving, any driver can believe that such a terrible event would never happen
to them or anyone they cared about.
The law of small numbers further
complicates this dynamic: statistics are not easily accessible to the general
population (10). While we may hear that 1 in 6 fatal car accidents in 2008 were
the result of distracted driving (18), if we do not have a face to put with
that one fatality, we can lose site of the life lost. As teens certainly know
many of their peers who have texted while driving and not gotten in to an
accident, their perception is severely altered in terms of the actual risks
associated with such a behavior (10). They know that they have texted behind
the wheel before and nothing has happened, so why would that ever change in the
future? If adolescents were able to hear true stories of other teens who had
died behind the wheel while reading or sending a text message, even if they did
not personally know these young men and women, they could begin to see the
consequences of a seemingly innocent action.
Further, using the Theory of Planned
Behavior to build a campaign designed for teenagers is irresponsible. As a
theory based on the rationality of the human decision-making process, it does
not address the fact that teenagers often act irrationally, speaking and acting
before they think. As described in numerous studies, adolescents are
experiencing a number of significant hormonal, physical and emotional changes
during this time in their life (19). It would be difficult to expect them to
rationally consider each thought before they acted on it when so much of their
lives is out of their control. Rather than focusing on the significance of
perceived behavioral control that a human being is assumed to have, a strong
intervention would tackle the most important element of a teen’s life: the
social network.
A novel intervention that employed the
strengths of current campaigns while focusing on appealing to the teenage
audience could successfully catch the ears and eyes of our youngest drivers. A
video campaign that shared the individual story of a family affected by their
teen texting while driving could help all young men and women grasp the
severity of taking such actions. This is not just an ad providing a statistic;
this is an ad introducing you to a real family who lost a beloved child. To
make this campaign even more powerful, each state in the country could have a
specific ad that told the tale of a teenager from their state. If a
Massachusetts teen were to see the face of one of their peers that perished in
a car accident in a town nearby, it would be difficult for them to ignore such
a story. This could be you. This could be your best friend. This could be your
cousin. By personalizing this issue, teens can no longer believe that accidents
caused by distracted driving are isolated events that occur outside of their
worlds. These advertisements could air on television channels frequented by
teenagers, such as MTV and E!
AT&T has headlined an advertising
campaign entitled “It Can Wait,” giving the individual stories of real
teenagers and the last texts they sent before they were killed in motor vehicle
accidents (20). The families of these teens detail their last moments alive, bravely
demonstrating the pain that is left behind after a loved one has passed away.
The idea behind this intervention is simple: look back at the last text message
you sent or received and read it out loud. Is this message worth getting in an
accident over? (20)
This type of ad campaign lowers reactance
and optimistic bias because the source of the message is someone that the
target audience of teenagers can relate to. An adult is not just listing all
the sobering statistics associated with driving while texting. A teen viewer
can relate to the source and understand that such a tragedy could happen to
them or one of their friends if they continue to drive while distracted. By
giving the exact words of the text the driver was sending or reading when they
crashed, the campaign is being as explicit as possible; “is this text worth
losing your life over?” One of the examples given depicts a beautiful teenage
girl who was driving to meet a boy the day before her high school graduation.
The last text she received read “where u at?” (20). Such an insignificant message
would hopefully cause teenagers to pause and consider their own decisions when
they are reaching to answer their cell phone during their next drive.
Such a concept gives control back to the
teens in the target audience. This use of the Illusion of Control understands
that when people make their own choices, there is an illusion that they control
their own fate (10). It becomes their choice whether it is necessary to read
that incoming message immediately or whether it can wait until the car is in
park. As so much in the lives of teenagers is out of their control, this can be
used as an opportunity to have some say in what happens to them. They can make
the decision that the incoming text message is not as important as getting to
their destination safely. Through AT&T’s campaign, a teenager can begin to
see and grasp the realities of distracted driving in a way that straight facts
do not provide while allowing them to take responsibility for their own
decisions.
Finally, if the Theory of Planned
Behavior cannot be successfully used to develop this campaign, another social
and behavioral science principle can be utilized to intervene at the most
vulnerable level. A teen is nothing without their social network, or so they
may believe. How can we effectively target the power of the teen social network
without losing sight of the final goal to erase texting while driving?
The idea behind the Social Networking
Theory is that people and events are interdependent and it is immensely arduous
to change the behavior of one individual when the rest of their environment
does not change (21). Whether a study looks at cell phone use in a group of
teenage peers or obesity in the Framingham Heart Study (21), it is obvious that
an individual is influenced by the family and friends that surround them. By
intervening at a group level, the focus is on social norms rather than
individual actions (10). It is more feasible to change the behaviors of a
teenager if their peers are also being challenged to think about the hazards of
their actions.
Distraction.gov understands the
importance of social media in the lives of adolescents by encouraging them to
reach out to their peers and tell them about the dangers of distracted driving.
However, the website still relies on the Theory of Planned Behavior, believing
that if a teen driver knows the facts, they will make the correct decision to
not text while driving. The Social Networking Theory is a much better fit for
such a targeted intervention, where the entire adolescent population is the
desired audience.
While engaging the principles used in the
most recent campaign by AT&T, a successful intervention should go one step
further, taking advantage of the Facebook, Twitter and Tumblr scene. An
intervention could partner with Facebook or Twitter, sponsoring ads that
plaster the sides of the website that teenagers visit every day; in fact, the Kaiser Family Foundation Report on
Media Use recently found that 53% of 15–18 year olds use a social
networking website daily (22). This would ensure that over 50% of the target
audience could be exposed to the message at least once every 24 hours.
Furthermore, this campaign would
elicit help from celebrities who are widely recognized by the teen demographic.
Placing these well-know public figures in the print ads would catch the eye of
the teen viewer, hopefully encouraging them to pause and read the words that
the celebrity is supporting.
Conclusion
In
an era when the average teen is sending 3,417 texts per month (23), it is obvious
that we are not going to change adolescent reliance on text messaging. As an
activity that has become so strongly engrained in our culture, we must find
ways of ensuring that teens are texting in the right place and at the right
time. This
intervention would combat the main criticisms found with the Distraction.gov
campaign. It would decrease psychological reactance by emphasizing the power
teens have in making such important behavioral decisions. It would limit the
optimistic bias, as the individual stories of actual teens killed in accidents
related to texting while driving provide faces to go along with the powerful
statistics and facts. By
developing an intervention derived from the power of Social Networking Theory,
we can reach teenagers in a way that is less offensive and overpowering than
the methods currently in place. A campaign portraying real teenagers and their
deaths behind the wheel can remind our young drivers that life is fleeting.
Forget the statistics and facts; this can happen to you. Getting respected
celebrities on board can help prove to these young men and women that
distracted driving is a real concern that everyone needs to be aware of. While
this intervention would not necessarily reach all the adolescents who currently
text and drive, it could begin to open the eyes of our vulnerable young driver
population. We are not alone in this
goal; as we strive to create more effective public health campaigns, wireless
communication companies are designing devices that can disable drivers’ cell
phones when they are in moving vehicles (24). In the future, we may have such
technology that completely We are not going to change the dependence on cell
phones and text messaging but we can hope to change the culture that goes along
with it.
References
Garner, Annie A., Philip R. Fine, Crystal A. Franklin,
Richard W. Sattin, and Despina Stavrinos. "Distracted Driving among
Adolescents: Challenges and Opportunities." Injury Prevention 17
(2011): 285. Injury Prevention, 27 June 2011. Web. 13 Apr. 2012.
<http://injuryprevention.bmj.com/content/early/2011/06/26/injuryprev-2011-040096.extract>.
2. "NHTSA, Ad Council Partner for New Campaign against
Texting While Driving." Welcome to the FastLane. U.S. Secretary of
Transportation, 28 Oct. 2011. Web. 16 Apr. 2012.
<http://fastlane.dot.gov/2011/10/3-new-psas-against-texting-while-driving.html>.
3. Strayer, David L., Frank A. Drews, and Dennis J. Crouch.
"A Comparison of the Cell Phone Driver and the Drunk Driver." Human
Factors 48 (2006): 381-91. Summer 2006. Web. 21 Apr. 2012.
<http://www.distraction.gov/research/PDF-Files/Comparison-of-CellPhone-Driver-Drunk-Driver.pdf>.
4. Atchley, Paul, Stephanie Atwood, and Aaron Boulton.
"The Choice to Text and Drive in Younger Drivers: Behavior May Shape
Attitude." Accident Analysis and Prevention 43 (2011): 134-42.
Print.
5. "What Is Distracted Driving?" Distracted
Driving. U.S. Department of Transportation. Web. 16 Apr. 2012.
<http://www.distraction.gov/content/get-the-facts/facts-and-statistics.html>.
6. "Cellphone and Texting Laws." Insurance
Institute for Highway Safety, Apr. 2012. Web. 6 Apr. 2012.
7. Brochure. Distracted Driving. U.S. Department of
Transporation. <www.distraction.gov>
8. Laurin, Kristin, Aaron C. Kay, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons.
"Reactance Versus Rationalization: Divergent Responses to Policies That
Constrain Freedom." Psychological Science 23 (2012): 205-09. 2 Feb.
2012. Web. 13 Apr. 2012. <http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/news/releases/people-rationalize-situations-theyre-stuck-with-but-rebel-when-they-think-theres-an-out.html>.
9. Silvia, Paul J. "Deflecting Reactance: The Role of
Similarity in Increasing Compliance and Reducing Resistance." Basic and
Applied Social Psychology 27.3 (2005): 277-84. Print.
10. Siegel,
Michael. Lecture. 12 April 2012.
11. Weinstein,
Neil D. "Unrealistic Optimism about Future Life Events." Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 39.5 (1980): 806-20. Print.
12. Harrison, Marissa A. "College Students’ Prevalence
and Perceptions of Text Messaging While Driving." Accident Analysis
& Prevention 43.4 (2011): 1516-520. Print.
13. Edberg, Mark. "Individual Health Behavior
Theories." Essentials of Health Behavior: Social and Behavioral Theory
in Public Health. Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett, 2007. 35-49. Print.
14. Nemme, HE and KM
White. Texting and Driving: Psychosocial Influences on Young People’s Texting
Intentions and Behavior. Accidental Analysis Prevention 2010 42(4):1257-65.
15. "Take the Pledge."
Distraction.gov. Web. 21 Apr. 2012.
<http://www.distraction.gov/download/Distraction_Pledge.pdf>.
16. "Teens: Take the Distracted
Driving Challenge." Distraction.gov. Web. 21 Apr. 2012. <http://www.distraction.gov/content/challenge.html>.
17. "Get Involved: Teens." Distraction.gov. Web.
21 Apr. 2012.
<http://www.distraction.gov/content/get-involved/teens.html>.
18. Wilson, Fernando A., and Jim P. Simpson. "Trends in
Fatalities From Distracted Driving in the United States, 1999 to 2008." American
Journal of Public Health 100 (2010): 2213-2219. Print.
19. Akcan, Ramazan, M. Mustafa Arslan, Necmi Çekin, and
Ramazan Karanfil. "Unexpected Suicide and Irrational Thinking in
Adolescence: A Case Report." Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine
18 (2011): 288-90. Print.
20 "Dangers of Text Messaging and Driving." Text
Messaging and Driving. AT&T. Web. 24 Apr. 2012.
<http://www.att.com/gen/press-room?pid=2964>.
21. Christakis, N. A. "Social Networks and Collateral
Health Effects." British Medical Journal 329.7459 (2004): 184-85.
Print.
22. Rideout,
R.J, Foehr, U.G, Roberts, D.F. “Generation M2 media in the lives of 8-to 18-year-olds.”Henry
J. Kaiser Family Foundation. 2010. <http://www.kff.org/entmedia/upload/8010.pdf>.
23.
"New Mobile Obsession: U.S. Teens
Triple Data Usage | Nielsen Wire." Nielsen Wire. Nielsen, 15 Dec.
2011. Web. 22 Apr. 2012. <http://blog.nielsen.com/nielsenwire/online_mobile/new-mobile-obsession-u-s-teens-triple-data-usage/>.
Labels: Adolescent Health, Cultural Issues, Health Communication, Red
0 Comments:
Post a Comment
Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]
<< Home